If you've been following
the blog recently, you'll know that Sue and I have been having some lively
discussions in the comments of the previous blogpost that related to autism and
loneliness. One of the questions Sue asked me was why I thought mastery
of key developmental milestones is absent or delayed in autism.
I undertook to answer this
by quoting from an article I wrote that was published in the UK journal 'Good
Autism Practice'. The citation for the full article in case anyone wants
to read it is: 'Why a developmental approach is crucial in supporting children
and adults with autism: using the principles and practice of Relationship
Development Intervention within an alternative school setting' Good Autism
Practice Volume 14, issue 1, May 2013.
Here is the excerpt:
'In autism, something interferes with
brain development early in life which derails the typical developmental process. In typical development, parents establish patterns
which the baby comes to rely on: e.g. right after birth: the pattern of feed,
rock, sleep, change; after a few weeks: the pattern of feed, rock, sleep,
change, play. The baby comes to know
that when s/he cries, the parent will come and take whatever action is
appropriate to sooth him and/or provide him with stimulation. In this way, the baby learns to trust the
pattern and the associated adult.
Parents adjust their response in relation to the feedback they get from
the baby. If the baby is still unhappy when
she has been fed, the parent tries winding her to see if this will settle her. If this doesn’t work, the parent tries
rocking her, playing with her etc. until she settles.
These patterns also set
the stage for emotional self-regulation: the baby learns that if s/he is upset,
the adult will provide comfort.
Emotional regulation is a process that starts out as a shared activity
between two or more people (co-regulation) and becomes internalised as the
child develops.
Even before talking develops,
the toddler has become a master at reading and responding to non-verbal
communication such as facial expression, gesture and prosody. Through hours of play and experimentation
with the parent, the toddler has already learned to reference the adult’s
emotional expression for information, including information about what to do
when s/he is faced with uncertainty.
This can be seen clearly
in the ‘visual cliff’ experiment
where the baby is unsure whether or not to cross what looks like a steep drop
to obtain a colourful toy.
To help decide what to do,
the baby references her mother (looks to her for information). When greeted
with an anxious face, the baby withdraws, but when greeted with a positive,
cheerful face, the baby proceeds to cross the visual cliff.
Children with autism fail
to develop this experience-sharing type of referencing because they have veered
away from the typical path of development where interpersonal engagement acts
as the ‘cradle’ for the development of higher-level ‘dynamic intelligence’ thinking
skills. This explains why children with
autism avoid change and uncertainty and why change and uncertainty lead to such
high levels of anxiety in autism.
In early development, the
adult is the ‘guide’ to the child’s ‘apprentice’ in a process called ‘guided
participation’ (Rogoff, 1990). Guided
participation enables the child to collaborate in a learning relationship with
close adults which is:
‘…..carried out day after
day, without much deliberation or conscious awareness. Children participate
alongside family and community members in authentic activities as apprentices,
actively seeking to appropriate meaning and expertise from adult guides who
provide opportunities for the apprentice to safely encounter cognitive
challenges.’
In babyhood and more so as
the baby matures into a toddler, parents pitch small challenges just at their
child’s current edge of competence: Vygotsky’s ‘zone of proximal
development’. The toddler’s feedback
enables the parent to decide whether the challenge has been pitched at the
right level. If it is too great, the
parent will scaffold the activity to make it easier. The toddler then successfully completes the
activity and, in experiencing success (which is usually spotlighted by the
parent with a comment such as ‘wow – you did it!’ and a big smile), lays down
an episodic memory of competence.
In typical development,
success that is spotlighted by the guide leads to an episodic memory (a personal,
autobiographical memory) being encoded.
These special memories of competence build up in a memory bank to be
used again when we are faced with another uncertainty. For instance, I have a condition called
‘topographical agnosia’ which basically means I can’t navigate my way out of a
paper bag. I quite often have to drive
to places I don’t know, which causes me anxiety. However, I am not daunted by this because I
can draw on my memories of previous success in similar uncertain
situations. I can remember successfully
using my satnav, stopping to ask people directions, phoning my husband to ask
him to look at the map and tell me where to go next. These memories have built my resilience,
which enables me to cope and to manage my anxiety, so also helping me to
regulate myself.
The crucial points here
are that typically developing children safely encounter cognitive challenges
during their interaction with the adult guide and that they lay down episodic
memories of competence thanks to the interpersonal engagement that takes place within the guided participation relationship. The experience (and memory) of success in
overcoming a challenge is partly what motivates them to continue being
curious. The child is successful because
the challenge is taking place within the interpersonal relatedness that is the
cornerstone of the guided participation relationship – so if the child
apprentice starts to falter, the adult guide intuitively picks up the signals
and scaffolds the activity to make it easier for the child to master the
challenge successfully.
Children with autism do
not have the safety net of their communication partner’s perspective to use as
a point of reference when faced with uncertainty. Autism has interfered with the child’s
development, so the child neither references for information nor gives feedback
to the parent. The parent is then unable
to take their role as the guide and consequently the child lays down few
episodic memories of competence in their ‘resilience repository’. For children with autism, their experiences
are more often those of failure, causing frustration, distress, withdrawal and
sometimes ‘meltdown’'.
To summarise: humans are
hard-wired to be social. The
evolutionary advantage of being social is that interpersonal relatedness within
the guided participation relationship is the vehicle that enables the
development of higher level thinking skills. Autism interferes with typical
brain development (with the hard-wiring to be social). The feedback loop between parent/caregiver
and child breaks down, the guided participation relationship is not in place
and so the interpersonal relatedness that leads to the development of higher
level thinking skills via mastery of key developmental milestones (referencing,
joint attention, co-regulation, self-regulation....and many more) cannot take place.